The mystery of why babies kick in the womb has finally been solved by scientists who say it helps their development.
Apparently, random movements boost the development of the sensorimotor system - such as hand-eye coordination, according to new research.
Right from birth - and even before - infants start to kick, wiggle and move seemingly without aim or external stimulation.
A kick can carry a force of more than 10 pounds and has mystified scientists for centuries. Now a model shows it helps the infant learn to control its body.
The discovery has implications for medical conditions and the development of more agile machines.
A Japanese team combined detailed motion capture of newborns and infants with a musculoskeletal computer model.
It enabled them to analyze communication among muscles and sensations across the whole body.
They found patterns of muscle interaction developing based on the babies' random exploratory behavior. It later helped them to perform sequential movements.
The discovery has implications for medical conditions. It sheds fresh light on a host of disorders.
They range from cramps and spasms to multiple sclerosis, spinal cord injury, motor neuron disease and even cerebral palsy.
The hundreds of neurons that control each muscle are synchronized in the fetus to create strong contractions that activate 'sensors.'
A better understanding of the sensorimotor system development could lead to earlier diagnoses - and more effective treatments.
Currently, there is limited knowledge about how babies learn to move their bodies.
(ESB Professional via Shutterstock)
Dr. Hoshinori Kanazawa, of the University of Tokyo and a co-author of the study, said: "Previous research into sensorimotor development has focused on kinematic properties, muscle activities which cause movement in a joint or a part of the body.
"However, our study focused on muscle activity and sensory input signals for the whole body.
"By combining a musculoskeletal model and neuroscientific method, we found spontaneous movements, which seem to have no explicit task or purpose, contribute to coordinated sensorimotor development."
The findings are based on recordings of the joint movements of 12 healthy newborns less than 10 days old and 10 young infants aged about three months.
Muscle activity and sensory input signals were estimated using the infant-scale musculoskeletal computer model of the whole body.
Computer algorithms analyze space and time, or 'spatiotemporal,' features of the interactions.
Dr. Kanazawa added: "We were surprised during spontaneous movement, infants' movements 'wandered' and they pursued various sensorimotor interactions.
"We named this phenomenon ‘sensorimotor wandering.' It has been commonly assumed sensorimotor system development generally depends on the occurrence of repeated sensorimotor interactions, meaning the more you do the same action the more likely you are to learn and remember it.
"However, our results implied infants develop their own sensorimotor system based on explorational behavior or curiosity, so they are not just repeating the same action but a variety of actions.
"In addition to this, our findings provide a conceptual linkage between early spontaneous movements and spontaneous neuronal activity."
Previous studies on humans and animals have shown that motor behavior involves a small set of primitive muscular control patterns.
They can typically be seen in task-specific or cyclic movements, like walking or reaching.
The latest results support the theory newborns and infants can acquire coordination skills through spontaneous whole-body movements without an explicit purpose or task.
Even through "sensorimotor wandering," the babies showed an increase in coordinated whole-body and anticipatory movements.
Infants showed more common patterns and sequential movements compared to the random movements of the newborn group.
Kanazawa is now planning to look at how sensorimotor wandering affects later development, such as walking and reaching, along with more complex behaviors and higher cognitive functions.
He added: "My original background is in infant rehabilitation. My big goal through my research is to understand the underlying mechanisms of early motor development and to find the knowledge that will help to promote baby development."
Most pregnant women begin to feel their baby move between 16 and 24 weeks, and the movements can be described as anything from a kick, flutter, swish or roll.
Stories and infographics by ‘Talker Research’ are available to download & ready to use. Stories and videos by ‘Talker News’ are managed by SWNS. To license content for editorial or commercial use and to see the full scope of SWNS content, please email [email protected] or submit an inquiry via our contact form.
We use cookies on our website to give you the most relevant experience by remembering your preferences and repeat visits. By clicking “Accept All”, you consent to the use of ALL the cookies. However, you may visit "Cookie Settings" to provide a controlled consent.
This website uses cookies to improve your experience while you navigate through the website. Out of these, the cookies that are categorized as necessary are stored on your browser as they are essential for the working of basic functionalities of the website. We also use third-party cookies that help us analyze and understand how you use this website. These cookies will be stored in your browser only with your consent. You also have the option to opt-out of these cookies. But opting out of some of these cookies may affect your browsing experience.
Necessary cookies are absolutely essential for the website to function properly. These cookies ensure basic functionalities and security features of the website, anonymously.
Cookie
Duration
Description
cookielawinfo-checkbox-analytics
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Analytics".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-functional
11 months
The cookie is set by GDPR cookie consent to record the user consent for the cookies in the category "Functional".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-necessary
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookies is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Necessary".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-others
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Other.
cookielawinfo-checkbox-performance
11 months
This cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Performance".
viewed_cookie_policy
11 months
The cookie is set by the GDPR Cookie Consent plugin and is used to store whether or not user has consented to the use of cookies. It does not store any personal data.
Functional cookies help to perform certain functionalities like sharing the content of the website on social media platforms, collect feedbacks, and other third-party features.
Performance cookies are used to understand and analyze the key performance indexes of the website which helps in delivering a better user experience for the visitors.
Analytical cookies are used to understand how visitors interact with the website. These cookies help provide information on metrics the number of visitors, bounce rate, traffic source, etc.
Advertisement cookies are used to provide visitors with relevant ads and marketing campaigns. These cookies track visitors across websites and collect information to provide customized ads.